A pink aleurone color, varying in
intensity and giving a mottled appearance, was observed in stocks of corn
homozygous a1a1 and heterozygous Dtdt. Crosses made in the summer of 1946, in the course
of other studies with this stock, indicated an unusual mode of inheritance of
this color factor called flush.
In general, of 105 ears examined, all
were either 100% flush, 100% colorless, or segregating 1:1. Selfed plants from
flush kernels of segregating ears had two classes of ears; some were altogether
colored, and others segregated 1:1. Selfed plants fron colorless kernels of
these same segregating ears also had two classes of ears; some colorless and
some segregating 1:1.
Further crosses made in the summer of
1947 indicated the same pattern of inheritance and especially the total lack of
effect of the male parent in determining the phenotype of the offspring.
When the female parent is homozygous
for flush, all offspring kernels will be colored regardless of the male
constitution, provided only that it comes from the same stock. When the female
parent is homozygous colorless, all offspring kernels are colorless. When the
female parent is heterozygous, all ears segregate 1:1. However, let us consider
the genotype of these kernels. When a heterozygous plant is selfed, half the
colored kernels of the offspring ear will be homozygous flush and half
heterozygous. Half the colorless kernels will also be heterozygous for flush
and half homozygous colorless. All these cases have been observed.
The data indicate that the expression of flush
depends upon the presence of the allele for color in at least two of the three
loci present in each aleurone cell. There is no dominance. This mode of
inheritance resembles that of floury.
Linkage tests are in progress. Expression of flush
is independent of Dt. It follows
then that the aleurone color factors A2 C and R
are all homozygous dominant. When homozygous flush plants were used as female
times a1‑et
tester stock, the offspring ears were all colorless, indicating the male carried
some factor inhibiting expression of flush. However, when sib females were
crossed to C.497 a1
tester, some of the kernels on the offspring ear were pigmented faintly.
The variability in intensity of color seems to be
affected greatly by both environmental conditions and modifiers. Careful
grading of color intensity of several ears individually gave in each case a
fairly typical normal distribration range with no appearance of sharp
discontinuities or classes. When a pale flush heterozygote female was crossed
to a deep flush male, the colored kernels on the offspring ear were, on the
average, deeper in color than were the colored kernels on ears from crosses of
sib females to pale flush males. Thus it appears that the male parent carries
modifiers that can affect the intensity of pigmentation, but the male cannot
determine whether or not the color will be present.
Under controlled environmental conditions this
factor may be useful for a quantitative study of modifiers, and further work is
in progress.
Ruth Sager